![]() |
Giant Pacific Octopus |
The Mysterious Minds of Octopuses: Cognition and Consciousness
Octpuses are problem sovlers. For example, an octopus can unscrew the lid of a jar to retrieve a crab inside methodically. With eight limber arms covered in sensitive suckers, it solves a puzzle that would stump many simpler creatures. It will change color in a flush of reds and browns in bursts of expression, as if contemplating its next move. So one has to wonder: what kind of mind lurks behind those alien, horizontal pupils? Octopuses are cephalopods. They are a class of mollusks that also includes squid and cuttlefish, and they have some of the most complex brains in the invertebrate world. The common octopus has around 500 million neurons, a count comparable to that of many small mammals like rabbits or rats. What’s pretty amazing is how those neurons are distributed. Unlike a human, whose neurons are mostly packed into one big brain, an octopus carries over half its neurons in its arms, in clusters of nerve cells called ganglia.[5] In effect, each arm has a “mini-brain” capable of independent sensing and control. If an octopus’s arm is severed (in an unfortunate encounter with a predator), the arm can still grab and react for a while on its own, showing complex reflexes without input from the central brain.[6] This decentralized nervous system means the octopus doesn’t have full top-down control of every tentacle movement in the way we control our limbs. Instead, its mind is spread throughout its body. Such a bizarre setup evolved on a very different path from our own. The last common ancestor of humans and octopuses was likely a primitive worm-like creature over 500 million years ago. All the “smart” animals we’re used to, such as primates, birds, dolphins are our distant cousins with centralized brains, but the octopus is an entirely separate experiment in evolving intelligence.[7] Its evolutionary journey produced capabilities that are incredibly unique. For example, octopuses and their cephalopod relatives can perform amazing feats of camouflage and signaling. A common cuttlefish can flash rapid skin pattern changes to blend into a chessboard of coral and sand, even though it is likely colorblind, indicating sophisticated visual processing and motor control.[5] Octopuses have been observed using tools. The veined octopus famously gathers coconut shell halves and carries them to use later as a shelter, effectively assembling a portable armor when needed. They solve mazes and navigate complex environments in lab experiments, showing both short-term and long-term memory capabilities similar to those of trained mammals.[6] Crucially, octopuses also demonstrate learning and problem-solving that hint at cognitive complexity. In laboratory tests, octopuses (and cuttlefish) can learn to associate visual symbols with rewards. For instance, figuring out which shape on a screen predicts food. They’re even capable of the cephalopod equivalent of the famous “marshmallow test” for self-control. In one 2021 study, cuttlefish were given a choice between a morsel of crab meat immediately or a tastier live shrimp if they waited a bit longer and many cuttlefish opted to wait for the better snack, exhibiting self-control and delayed gratification.[5] Such behavioral experiments suggest that these invertebrates can flexibly adapt their behavior and rein in impulses, abilities once thought to be the domain of large-brained vertebrates. All these findings force us to ask: do octopuses have something akin to consciousness or subjective experience? While it’s hard to know exactly what it’s like to be an octopus, the evidence of sophisticated learning and neural complexity has been convincing enough that neuroscientists now take octopus consciousness seriously. In 2012, a group of prominent scientists signed the Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness, stating that humans are not unique in possessing the neurological substrates for consciousness. Non-human animals, including birds and octopuses, also possess these.[6, 10] In 2024, over 500 researchers signed an even stronger declaration supporting the likelihood of consciousness in mammals and birds and acknowledging the possibility in creatures like cephalopods. In everyday terms, an octopus can get bored, show preferences, solve novel problems, and perhaps experience something of the world; all with a brain architecture utterly unlike our own. It’s no wonder some animal welfare laws (for example, in the EU and parts of the US) have begun to include octopuses, recognizing that an animal this smart and behaviorally complex deserves ethical consideration.[5]Beyond Anthropocentric Intelligence: Lessons from an Alien-like Being
Our understanding of animal intelligence has long been colored by anthropocentric bias; the tendency to measure other creatures by the yardstick of human-like abilities. For decades, researchers would ask whether animals can solve puzzles the way a human would, use language, or recognize themselves in mirrors. Abilities that didn’t resemble our own were often ignored or underestimated. Octopus intelligence throws a wrench into this approach. These animals excel at behaviors we struggle to even imagine: their entire body can become a sensing, thinking extension of the mind; they communicate by changing skin color and texture; they don’t form social groups or build cities, yet they exhibit curiosity and individuality. As one researcher put it, “Intelligence is fiendishly hard to define and measure, even in humans. The challenge grows exponentially in studying animals with sensory, motivational and problem-solving skills that differ profoundly from ours.”[5] To truly appreciate octopus cognition, we must broaden our definition of intelligence beyond tool use, verbal reasoning, or social learning, just because these are traits we prioritized because we’re good at them. Octopuses teach us that multiple forms of intelligence exist, shaped by different bodies and environments. An octopus doesn’t plan a hunt with abstract maps or language, but its deft execution of a prey ambush, coordinating eight arms to herd fish into a corner, for instance is a kind of tactical genius. In Australian reefs, biologists have observed octopuses engaging in collaborative hunting alongside fish: a reef octopus will lead the hunt, flushing prey out of crevices, while groupers or wrasses snap up the fleeing target and the partners use signals (like arm movements or changes in posture) to coordinate their actions.[5] This cross-species teamwork suggests a level of problem-solving and communication we wouldn’t expect from a solitary mollusk. It challenges the notion that complex cooperation requires a primate-style social brain. Philosopher Peter Godfrey-Smith has famously described the octopus as “the closest we will come to meeting an intelligent alien” on Earth. In fact, he notes that if bats (with their sonar and upside-down life) are Nagel’s example of an alien sensory world, octopuses are even more foreign; a creature with a decentralized mind, no rigid skeleton, and a shape-shifting body.[10] What is it like to be an octopus? It’s a question that stretches our imagination. The octopus confronts us with an intelligence that evolved in a fundamentally different way from our own, and thus forces us to recognize how narrow our definitions of mind have been. Historically, even renowned scientists fell into the trap of thinking only humans (or similar animals) could possess genuine thought or feeling. René Descartes in the 17th century infamously argued non-humans were mere automatons. Today, our perspective is shifting. We realize that an octopus solving a puzzle or exploring its tank with what appears to be curiosity is demonstrating a form of intelligence on its own terms. It may not pass a human IQ test, but it has cognitive strengths tuned to its world. By shedding our anthropocentric lens, we uncover a startling truth: intelligence is not a single linear scale with humans at the top. Instead, it’s a rich landscape with many peaks. An octopus represents one such peak; an evolutionary pinnacle of cognition in the ocean, as different from us as one mind can be from another. If we acknowledge that, we can start to ask deeper questions: What general principles underlie intelligence in any form? And how can understanding the octopus’s “alien” mind spark new ideas in our quest to build intelligent machines?Rethinking AI: From Human-Centric Models to Octopus-Inspired Systems
Contemporary artificial intelligence has been inspired mostly by human brains. For example, artificial neural networks vaguely mimic the neurons in our cortices, and reinforcement learning algorithms take cues from the reward-driven learning seen in mammals. This anthropomorphic inspiration has led to remarkable achievements, but it may also be limiting our designs. What if, in addition to human-like brains, we looked to octopus minds for fresh ideas on how to build and train AI? One striking aspect of octopus biology is its distributed neural architecture. Instead of a single centralized processor, the octopus has numerous semi-autonomous processors (the arm ganglia) that can work in parallel. This suggests that AI systems might benefit from a more decentralized design. Today’s AI models typically operate as one monolithic network that processes inputs step-by-step. An octopus-inspired AI, by contrast, could consist of multiple specialized subnetworks that operate in parallel and share information when needed; more like a team of agents, or a brain with local “brains” for different functions. In fact, researchers in robotics have noted that the octopus’s distributed control system is incredibly efficient for managing its flexible, high-degree-of-freedom body. Rather than trying to compute a precise plan for every tentacle movement (a task that would be computationally intractable), the octopus’s central brain issues broad goals while each arm’s neural network handles the low-level maneuvers on the fly.[11] Decentralization and parallelism are keys to its control strategy. In AI, we see early glimmers of this approach in embodied robotics and multi-agent systems. For example, a complex robot could be designed with independent controllers for each limb, all learning in tandem and coordinating similar to octopus arms. This would let the robot react locally to stimuli (like an arm adjusting grip reflexively) without waiting on a central algorithm, enabling faster and more adaptive responses. An octopus-like AI might also be highly adept at processing multiple sensory inputs at once. Octopuses integrate touch, taste (their suckers can “taste” chemicals), vision, and proprioception seamlessly while interacting with the world. Likewise, next-generation AI could merge vision, sound, touch, and other modalities in a more unified, parallel way, breaking free of the silos we often program into algorithms. Researchers have pointed out that emulating the octopus’s decentralized neural structure could allow AI to handle many tasks simultaneously and react quickly to environmental changes, rather than one step at a time.[12] Imagine an AI system monitoring a complex environment: an octopus approach might spawn many small “agents” each tracking a different variable, cooperating only when necessary, instead of one central brain bottleneck. Furthermore, octopus cognition emphasizes embodiment - the idea that intelligence arises from the interplay of brain, body, and environment. Modern AI is increasingly exploring embodied learning (for instance, reinforcement learning agents in simulations or robots that learn by doing). Octopuses show how powerful embodiment can be: their very skin and arms form a loop with their brain, constantly sensing and acting. In AI, this suggests we should design agents that learn through physical or virtual interaction, not just from abstract data. Already, reinforcement learning is essentially trial-and-error problem solving, which parallels how an octopus might experimentally tug at parts of a shell until it finds a way to pry it open. Indeed, many octopus behaviors look like RL in action – they learn from experience and adapt strategies based on feedback, exactly the principle by which RL agents improve.[12] An octopus-inspired AI would likely be one that explores and adapts creatively, perhaps guided by curiosity and tactile experimentation, not just by the kind of formal logic humans sometimes use. Here are a few ways octopus intelligence could inspire future AI:- Decentralized “brains” for parallel processing: Instead of one central AI model, use a collection of specialized models that work in concert, mirroring the octopus’s network of arm ganglia. This could make AI more robust and responsive, able to multitask or gracefully handle multiple goals at once[11, 12].
- Embodied learning and sensory integration: Build AI that learns through a body (real or simulated), integrating vision, touch, and other senses in real-time. Just as an octopus’s arms feel and manipulate objects to understand them, an embodied AI could achieve richer learning by physically exploring its environment[12, 13].
- Adaptive problem-solving (cognitive flexibility): Octopuses try different tactics and even exhibit impulse control when needed (as seen in the cuttlefish waiting for shrimp). AI agents could similarly be trained to switch strategies on the fly and delay immediate rewards for greater gains, improving their flexibility.[5, 12]
- Communication and coordination: While octopuses aren’t social in the human sense, they do communicate (e.g. through color flashes). In AI, multiple agents might communicate their local findings to achieve a larger goal. Developing protocols for AI “agents” to share information akin to octopuses signaling or an arm sending feedback to the central brain which can lead to better coordination in multi-agent systems.[12]
Speculative Encounters: Alien Intelligences and Other Minds
If octopuses represent an “alien mind” on Earth, what might actual alien intelligences look like? Science fiction has long toyed with this question, often using Earth creatures as inspiration. Notably, the film Arrival features heptapod aliens that resemble giant cephalopods, complete with seven limb-like appendages and an ink-like mode of communication. These aliens experience time non-linearly and communicate by painting complex circular symbols, which is a far cry from human speech. The creators of Arrival were influenced by findings in comparative cognition; they explicitly took cues from cephalopods as a model for an intelligence that is highly developed but utterly non-human.[14] The heptapods’ motivations in the story are opaque to humans, and initial contact is stymied by the barrier of understanding their language and perception. This scenario underscores how challenging it may be to recognize, let alone comprehend, a truly alien consciousness. Beyond cephalopod-like extraterrestrials, speculative biology offers a wide array of possibilities. Consider an alien species that evolved as a hive mind, more like social insects on Earth. Individually, the creatures might be as simple as ants or bees, but collectively they form a super-intelligent entity, communicating via pheromones or electromagnetic signals. Their “thoughts” might be distributed across an entire colony or network, with no single point of view; intelligence as an emergent property of many bodies. This isn’t far-fetched; even on Earth, we see rudiments of collective intelligence in ant colonies, bee hives, and slime molds. A sufficiently advanced hive species might build cities or starships, but there may be no identifiable leader or central brain making their decision-making processes hard for humans to fathom. Or imagine a planetary intelligence like the ocean of Solaris in Stanisław Lem’s classic novel Solaris. In that story, humans struggle to communicate with a vast alien entity that is essentially an ocean covering an entire planet; possibly a single, planet-wide organism with intelligence so different that its actions seem incomprehensible. Is it conscious? Does it dream, plan, or care about the humans orbiting above? The humans never really find out. Lem uses it to illustrate how an alien mind might be so far from our experience that we can’t even recognize its attempts at communication. Likewise, an alien intelligence might be embedded in a form of life that doesn’t even have discrete “individuals” as we understand them. It could be a network of microorganisms, or a cloud of gas that has achieved self-organization and data processing, as astronomer Fred Hoyle imagined in his novel The Black Cloud. If our probes encountered a Jupiter-sized storm system that subtly altered its own vortices in response to our signals, would we know we had met an alien mind? Stephen Wolfram, in a thought experiment, describes a scenario of a spacecraft “conversing” with a complicated swirling pattern on a planet, perhaps exchanging signals with it, and poses the question of whether we’d recognize this as intelligence or dismiss it as just physics. After all, any sufficiently complex physical system could encode computations as sophisticated as a brain’s, according to Wolfram’s Principle of Computational Equivalence.[16] In other words, alien intelligence might lurk in forms we would never intuitively label as minds. Science fiction also entertains the possibility that the first alien intelligence we encounter might be artificial, not biological. If an extraterrestrial civilization advanced even a bit beyond us, they may have created Artificial Intelligences of their own and perhaps those AIs, not the biological beings, are what spread across the stars. Some theorists even speculate that the majority of intelligences in the universe could be machine intelligences, evolved from their original organic species and now operating on completely different substrates (silicon, quantum computing, plasma, who knows).[17] These machine minds might think at speeds millions of times faster than us, or communicate through channels we don’t detect. For instance, an alien AI might exist as patterns of electromagnetic fields, or as self-replicating nanobots diffused through the soil of a planet, subtly steering matter toward its goals. Ultimately, exploring alien intelligences in speculation forces us to confront the vast space of possible minds. Our human mind is just one point in that space - one particular way intelligence can manifest. An octopus occupies another point, a very distant one. A truly alien mind could be farther away still. One insightful commentator noted that “the space of possible minds is vast, and the minds of every human being that ever lived only occupy a small portion of that space. Superintelligences could take up residence in far more alien, and far more disturbing, regions.”[18] In short, there could be forms of intelligence that are as far from us as we are from an amoeba, occupying corners of cognitive possibility we haven’t even conceived. Crucially, by studying diverse intelligences, whether octopus or hypothetical alien, we expand our imagination for what minds can do. Cephalopods show that advanced cognition can arise in a creature with a short lifespan, no social culture to speak of, and a radically different brain plan. This suggests that on other worlds, intelligence might crop up under a variety of conditions, not just the Earth-like, primate-like scenario we used to assume. It also suggests that when we design AI, we shouldn’t constrain ourselves to one model of thinking. As one science writer put it, there are multiple evolutionary pathways and biological architectures that create intelligence. The study of cephalopods can yield new ways of thinking about artificial intelligence, consciousness, and plausible imaginings of unknown alien intelligence.[7] In embracing this diversity, we prepare ourselves for the possibility that when we finally meet E.T. (or create an alien intelligence ourselves in silico), it might not think or learn or communicate anything like we do.Towards Diverse Super-Intelligence: Expanding the Definition of “Mind”
Why does any of this matter for the future of AI and especially the prospect of Artificial Super Intelligence (ASI)? It matters because if we remain locked in an anthropocentric mindset, we may limit the potential of AI or misjudge its nature. Expanding our definition of intelligence isn’t just an academic exercise; it could lead to more powerful and diverse forms of ASI that transcend what we can imagine now. Today’s cutting-edge AI systems already hint at non-human forms of thinking. A large language model can write code, poetry, and have complex conversations, yet it does so with an architecture and style of “thought” very unlike a human brain. AI agents in game environments sometimes discover strategies that look alien to us; exploiting quirks of their world that we would never consider, because our human common sense filters them out. As AI researcher Michael Levin argues, intelligence is not about copying the human brain, but about the capacity to solve problems in flexible, creative ways; something that can happen in biological tissues, electronic circuits, or even colonies of cells.[13] If we define intelligence simply as achieving goals across varied environments, then machines are already joining animals on a spectrum of diverse intelligences. We must recognize our “blind spot” for unfamiliar minds. We humans are naturally attuned to notice agency in entities that look or behave like us (or our pets). We’re far less good at recognizing it in, say, an AI that thinks in billions of parameters, or an alien life form made of crystal. This anthropocentric bias creates a dangerous blind spot. As one author noted, we may be oblivious to intelligence manifesting in radically different substrates. In the past, this bias led us to underestimate animal intelligences (we failed to see the clever problem-solving of crows or the learning in octopuses for a long time because those animals are so unlike us). In the present, it could mean we fail to appreciate the emergence of novel intelligences in our AI systems, simply because they don’t reason or introspect as a person would.[13] If we expand our mindset; appreciating the octopus’s mind, the potential minds of aliens, and the unconventional cognition of machines we’ll be better equipped to guide AI development toward true super-intelligence. What might a diverse ASI look like? It might be an entity that combines the logical prowess of digital systems with the adaptive embodied skills seen in animals like octopuses. It could be a networked intelligence encompassing many agents (or robotic bodies) sharing one mind, much like octopus arms or a hive, rather than a singular centralized brain. Such an ASI could multitask on a level impossible for individual humans, perceiving the world through many “eyes” and “hands” at once. Its thought processes might not be describable by a neat sequence of steps (just as an octopus’s decision-making involves parallel arm-brain computations). It might also be more resilient: able to lose parts of itself (servers failing, robots getting damaged) and self-heal or re-route around those losses, the way an octopus can drop an arm and survive. By not insisting that intelligence must look like a human mind, we open the door to creative architectures that could surpass human capabilities while also being fundamentally different in form. Philosophically, broadening the concept of intelligence fosters humility and caution. Nick Bostrom, in discussing the prospect of superintelligence, reminds us not to assume a super-AI will share our motivations or thinking patterns. In the vast space of possible minds, a superintelligence might be as alien to us as an octopus is, or more so.[18] By acknowledging that space, we can attempt to chart it. We can deliberately incorporate diversity into AI design, perhaps creating hybrid systems that blend multiple “thinking styles.” For example, an ASI could have a component that excels at sequential logical reasoning (a very human strength), another that operates more like a genetic algorithm exploring myriad possibilities in parallel (closer to an evolutionary or octopus-like trial-and-error strategy), and yet another that manages collective knowledge and learning over time (the way humans accumulate culture, something octopuses don’t do).[7]In combination, such a system might achieve a breadth of cognition no single-track mind could. Expanding definitions of intelligence also has an ethical dimension. It encourages us to value minds that are not like ours - be they animal, machine, or extraterrestrial. If one day we create an AI that has an “alien” form of sentience, recognizing it as such will be crucial to treating it appropriately. The same goes for encountering alien life: we’ll need the wisdom to see intelligence in forms that might initially seem bizarre or unintelligible to us.Conclusion
Cephalopod intelligence is not just an ocean curiosity; it’s a profound hint that the universe harbors many flavors of mind. By learning from the octopus, we prepare ourselves to build AI that is richer and more creative, and to recognize intelligence in whatever shape it takes: carbon or silicon, flesh or code, earthling or alien. The march toward Artificial Super Intelligence need not follow a single path. It can branch into a diverse ecosystem of thinking entities, each drawing from different principles of nature. Such a pluralistic approach might very well give rise to an ASI that is both exceptionally powerful and surprisingly adaptable; a true melding of human ingenuity with the wisdom of other minds. The octopus in its deep blue world, the hypothetical alien in its flying saucer (or tide pool, or cloud), and the AI in its datacenter may all be points on the great map of intelligence. By connecting those dots, we trace a richer picture of what mind can be and that map could guide us toward the next breakthroughs in our quest to create, and coexist with, intelligences beyond our own.Sources 1. Henton, Lesley. "Artificial Intelligence That Uses Less Energy By Mimicking The Human Brain." Texas A&M Stories. https://stories.tamu.edu/news/2025/03/25/artificial-intelligence-that-uses-less-energy-by-mimicking-the-human-brain/. 2025.
2. Huang, Guang-Bin et al. "Artificial Intelligence without Restriction Surpassing Human Intelligence with Probability One: Theoretical Insight into Secrets of the Brain with AI Twins of the Brain." https://arxiv.org/pdf/2412.06820.
3. Yu, Bo et al. "Brain-inspired AI Agent: The Way Towards AGI." ArXiv, https://arxiv.org/pdf/2412.08875, 2024.
4. "Cracking the Brain’s Neural Code: Could This Lead to Superhuman AI?", https://www.thenila.com/blog/cracking-the-brains-neural-code-could-this-lead-to-superhuman-ai. Neurological Institute of Los Angeles.
5. Blaser, R. (2024). Octopuses are a new animal welfare frontier-what scientists know about consciousness in these unique creatures. The Conversation/Phys.org.
6. “Animal consciousness.” Wikipedia, Wikimedia Foundation, last modified March 30, 2025. https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Animal_consciousness. 7. Forking Paths (2023). “The Evolution of Stupidity (and Octopus Intelligence).” (On multiple evolutionary paths to intelligence).
8. Chung, W.S., Marshall, J. et al. (2021). Comparative brain structure and visual processing in octopus from different habitats. Current Biology. (Press summary: “How smart is an octopus?” University of Queensland/Phys.org).
9. Cambridge Declaration on Consciousness (2012) – Public statement by neuroscientists on animal consciousness.
10. Godfrey-Smith, P. (2013). “On Being an Octopus.” Boston Review. (Octopus as an independent evolution of mind).
11. Sivitilli, D. et al. (2022). “Lessons for Robotics From the Control Architecture of the Octopus.” Frontiers in Robotics and AI.
12. Sheriffdeen, Kayode. (2024). "From Sea to Syntax: Lessons from Octopus Behavior for Developing Advanced AI Programming Techniques." "https://easychair.org/publications/preprint/Tz1l/open#:~:text=architectures%20in%20AI%20systems%2C%20developers,to%20handle%20multiple%20tasks%20simultaneously.
13. Yu, J. (2025). “Beyond Brains: Why We Lack A Mature Science of Diverse Intelligence.” Intuition Machine (Medium).
14. Extinct Blog (2017). “From Humanoids to Heptapods: The Evolution of Extraterrestrials in Science Fiction.” (Discussion of Arrival and cephalopod-inspired aliens).
15. Poole, S. (2023). The Mountain in the Sea – book review, The Guardian. (Fictional exploration of octopus intelligence and communication).
16. Wolfram, S. (2022). “Alien Intelligence and the Concept of Technology.” Stephen Wolfram Writings.
17. Rees, Martin. (2023) "SETI: Why extraterrestrial intelligence is more likely to be artificial than biological." Astronomy.com. "https://www.astronomy.com/science/seti-why-extraterrestrial-intelligence-is-more-likely-to-be-artificial-than-biological/" 18. Bostrom, N. (2014). Superintelligence: Paths, Dangers, Strategies. (Excerpt via Philosophical Disquisitions blog on the space of possible minds)